Wednesday 24 September 2014

Gender Inequality: An Issue or an Individual's Choice?

By Hannah Ross-McAlpine


“Women have been taught that, for us, the earth is flat, and that if we venture out, we will fall off the edge-” Andrea Dworkin

If you live in country, like New Zealand, you are most likely enjoying some of the highest levels of gender equality in the world. We are a nation that has afforded women the right to vote, the right to make their own choices, to live independently and have legal protection from any gender based discrimination. By comparison, we've made strides that many countries only aspire to.

So why are some feminists still talking about gender inequality in New Zealand, surely any inequality we experience is a result of individual choice?

In order to answer this question one must turn their attention to gender stereotypes. There is a unique pervasiveness to gender stereotypes in comparison to virtually all other forms of stereotyping. Gender stereotypes tend to be prescriptive rather than descriptive; meaning they indicate and influence what many people believe men and women should be. But how influential can a few stereotypes like ‘women belong in the kitchen,’ actually be? Surely only a few women experience messages like these?


Well not quite, we are all exposed to gender stereotypes from a young age, most as early as the day they were born. Were you wrapped in a blue or pink blanket at the hospital?  These ideas about gender shape our identity. It is the stereotypes introduced in childhood that are reinforced throughout our lives and become self-fulfilling prophecies.

Gender stereotypes can be illustrated by asking individuals to describe a typical man and a typical woman. Across 30 different countries college students described the typical man as; adventurous, assertive, aggressive, independent and task orientated. In comparison a typical woman was described as: sensitive, gentle, dependent, emotional and people orientated.

Before their first birthday children are able to distinguish men from women and by the age of three they can identify their own gender, showing favor to it. As a result, they begin to form stereotypical beliefs and preferences about stories and toys. Furthermore, toddlers display surprise when adults behave in ways that are inconsistent with their gender role.  Although biology and evolution play a significant part, it is clear children learn and adopt stereotypes from their parents and other role models.

Beliefs about gender are so deeply ingrained that they influence the attitudes and behaviour of parents as soon as their baby is born. Researchers found that within 24 hours of birth parents saw their sons as stronger, larger, more alert and co-ordinated in comparison to girls who were seen as softer, smaller and more finely featured. Despite these differences in perception there were actually no differences between the newborns in height, weight or other aspects of physical appearance. Adding to this perception disconnect, it was found that mothers of young children underestimated their child’s crawling ability if their child was female and overestimated it if the child was male.

Unsurprisingly, this difference in perception is not limited to thoughts, subsequently it affects the behavior of parents too. Mothers are much more likely to interrupt their female children to warn them of physical risk than they are with their male children. By the age of six years old, all children, regardless of gender, believe that girls are at greater risk of injury than boys, yet this is not actually the case, statistically boys experience more injury than girls.

However it is not only parents that facilitate the persistence and perpetuation of gender stereotypes. Another very effective mechanism for perpetuating stereotypes is the media. The most pressing and perhaps dire concern about the existence of stereotypes in the media is how depictions can influence viewers, often without viewers knowing it. Studies indicate that men who have seen sexist commercials behave in ways that are more sexualised and treat women in objectifying ways, having been primed with images of women as sex objects on TV. Have you seen any of these commercials recently and would you recognise them as sexist?

Yet the consequences do not stop there, TV commercials that show stereotypical women have a significant impact on women who view them. Women watching such commercials were more likely to express lower self-confidence, less independence, and fewer career aspirations. They were even more likely to perform poorly on a math test in comparison to those who viewed counter stereotypical ads. How many of these have you seen in your life?

Recent research shows the work force also facilitates the persistence and pervasiveness of stereotypes. There is growing evidence that when presented with an equally qualified man and woman gender considerations enter into the decision making. Men are often the preferred candidates for ‘masculine’ jobs such as managing, whereas women are preferred for so called ‘feminine’ jobs such as being a receptionist.

One of the most pervasive and inhibiting gender stereotypes that women face in the workforce presents them with an impossible dilemma; women are seen as more competent if they present themselves with stereotypically masculine traits, yet when they do so they are also seen as less socially skilled and less attractive. Such perceptions may ultimately prevent women from achieving their career goals or gaining new jobs.

There are a number of studies that demonstrate that success and likeability are positively correlated for men and negatively correlated for women. When men are successful, they are liked by both genders however when women are similarly successful they are often viewed as selfish and both genders dislike such women. By behaving in ways necessary to achieve success in the workforce, women face severe social consequences. Yet when men behave ambitiously, assertively, confidently and achieve professional success they receive on-going positive reinforcement.  

The research discussed is merely a simple illustration of the numerous and ingrained gender stereotypes we experience. The research highlights distinct gender differences in how we experience the world and what we are taught is gender appropriate. These ‘gender lessons’ are persistent and perpetuated throughout our lives.

In light of these stereotypes we consciously and subconsciously create social, cultural, professional and emotional barriers that hinder our ability to achieve gender equality. These barriers mustn’t be dismissed as a woman’s individual choice. Just as stereotypes once facilitated a racist, segregated society, modern less obvious stereotypes significantly influence a woman’s ability to make choices. If such gender stereotypes are ignored and not challenged, we may never experience or know a society that has truly achieved gender equality.


References

Allport, G.W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Ashmore, R.D., & Del Boca, F.K. (1979). Sex stereotypes and implicit personality theory:    Toward a cognitive-social psychological conceptualisation. Sex Roles, 219-48

Barreto, M., Ryan, M.K. & Schmitt, M.T. (2009). The glass ceiling in the 21st century: Understanding barriers to gender equality. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Carnaghi, A., & Yzerbyt, V.Y. (2007) Subtyping and social consensus: The role of the audience in the maintenance of stereotypic beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 902-922.

Convention on the Elimination of all forms of Discrimination against Women. (1979). United Nations. Accessed 2nd August 2014 from: http://www.un.org/womenwatch/daw/cedaw/cedaw.htm

Cook, R.J. & Cusack, S. (2010). Gender stereotyping. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: University of Pennsylvania Press

Cuddy, A.J., Norton, M.L., & Fiske, S.T. (2004). This old stereotype: The pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 267-285.

Davis, P.G., Spencer, S.J., Quinn, D.M., & Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype threat can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1615-1628

Dunning, D., & Sherman, D.A. (1997). Stereotypes and tacit inference. Journal of Personality Psychology, 73, 459-471.

Eagly, A.H. (2004). Few women at the top: how role incongruity produces prejudice and the glass ceiling. In D. van Knippenberg & M. Hog (Eds). Leadership and power: Identity processes in groups and organisations. (pp. 79-93). London: Sage Publications

Glick, P., & Rudman, L.A. (2010). Sexism. In J.F. Dovidio, M. Hewstone, P. Glick & V.M. Esses (Eds). The Sage handbook of prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination. (pp. 328-345). London: Sage Publications

Heilman, M.E. & Okimoto, T.G. (2007). Why are women penalised for success at male tasks? The implied communality deficit. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1, 81-92

Hewstone, M., & Lord, C.G. (1998). Changing intergroup cognitions and intergroup behaviour: the role of typicality. In C. Sedikides, J. Schopler, & C.A. Insko (Eds), Intergroup cognition and intergroup behaviour (pp. 367-392). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum

Hilton, J.L. & von Hippel, W. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 231-271

Hoffmann, C. & Hurst, N. (1990). Gender Stereotypes: Perception or rationalisation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 197-208

Johnston, K.E., & Jacobs, J.E. (2003). Children’s illusory correlations: the role of attentional bias in group formation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 129-160

Kassin, S. M., Fein, S., & Markus, H. R. (2010). Social psychology (8th ed.). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Kmec. J.A. (2005). Setting occupational sex segregation in motion: sex traditional employment. Work and Occupations, 32, 322-354

Klein, H., & Shiffman, K. (2009). Underrepresentation and symbolic annihilation of socially disenfranchised groups (‘outgroups’) in animated cartoons. Howard Journal of Communications, 20, 55-71

Knobloch, S., Callison, C., Chen, L., Fritzsche, A., & Zillmann, D. (2005). Children’s sex stereotyped self-socialisation through selective exposure to entertainment: Cross-cultural experiments in German, China and the United States. Journal of Communication, 55, 122-138.

Krueger, J. (1998). On the perception fs social consensus. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 163-240

Kunda, Z., Sinclair, L., & Griffin, D. (1997). Equal ratings but separate meanings: Stereotypes and the construal of traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 720-734

Leinbach, M.D., & Fagot, B.I. (1993). Catergorical habituation to male and female faces: Gender schematic processing in infancy. Infant Behaviour and Development, 16, 317-332

Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

Marshell, B.L. (2000). Configuring gender: Explorations in theory and politics. Canada: Broadview Press

Martin, C.L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children’s gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66, 1453-1471

Meister, T. & Hewstone, M. (2006). Illusory and spurious correlations. Distinct phenomena or joint outcomes of exemplar-based-category learning? European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 315-336.

Messineo, M.J. (2008). Does advertising on Black Entertainment Television portray more positive gender representations compared to broadcast networks? Sex Roles, 59, 752-764

Mondschein, E.R., Adolph, K.E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C.S. (2000). Gender bias in mothers’ expectations about infant crawling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 304-316.

Morrongiello, B.A., & Dawber, T. (2000). Mothers’ responses to sons and daughters engaging in injury-risk behaviours on a playground: implications for sex differences in injury rates. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 89-103

Nassif, A., & Gunter, B. (2008). Gender representation in television advertisements in Britain and Saudi Arabia. Sex Roles, 58, 752-760

Newman, R.S, & Uleman, J.S. (1989). Spontaneous trait inference. In J.S. Uleman & J.A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 155-188). New York: Guilford.

Oakes, P.J. & Turner, J.C. (1990). Is limited information processing the cause of social stereotyping? In W. Stroebe &  M. Hewstone (Eds). European Review of Social Psychology 1, pp. 111-125). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Post, R.  (2000). Prejudicial appearances: The logic of American antidiscrimination law. California Law Review, 88, 1-49, pp. 18

Prentice, D.A. & Carranza, E. (2002). What women should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269-281.

Risen, J.L, Gilovich, T., & Dunning, D. (2007). One-shot illusory correlations and stereotype formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1492-1502

Rosenthal, R. (2002). Covert communication in classrooms, clinics, courtrooms and cubicles. American Psychologist, 57, 839-849

Rubin, J.Z., Provenzano, F.J. & Luria, Z. (1974). The eye of the beholder: Parents’ views on sex of newborns. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 44, 512-519

Ruble, D.N., & Martin, C.L. (1998). Gender development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 5th edition: Volume 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp.933-1016). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Rudman, L.A. & Borgida, E. (1995). The afterglow of construct accessibility: the behavioural consequences of priming men to view women as sexual objects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 493-517

Rudman, L.A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 743-762.

Sandberg, S. (2013). Lean in: women, work and the will to lead. Ebury Publishing

Serbin., L.A., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Eichstedt, J.A. (2002). Infants’ response to gender-inconsistent events. Infancy, 3, 531-542.

Slovic, P. (2000). The perception of risk. London: Earthscan

Stone, J., Perry, Z.W., & Darkey, J.M. (1997). “White men can’t jump”: Evidence for the perceptual confirmation of racial stereotypes following a basketball game. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 291-306.

Stroessner, S.J., & Plaks, J.E. (2001). Illusory correlation and stereotype formation: tracing the arc of research over a quarter century. In G.B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp.247-259). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Susskind, J. (2003) Children’s perception of gender-biased illusory correlations: Enhancing pre-existing relationships between gender and behaviour. Sex Roles, 48, 483-494

Tenenbaum, H.R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38, 615-630

Todorov, A., & Uleman, J.S. (2004). The person reference process in spontaneous trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 482-493

Ward, L.M., & Friedman, K. (2006). Using TV as a guide: Associations between television viewing and adolescents’ sexual attitudes and behaviour. Journal of Research on Adolescents, 16, 133-156

Williams, J.E. & Best, D.L. (1982). Measuring sex stereotypes: A thirty nation study. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage

World Health Organisation. (2014). Gender, woman and health. Accessed 1st August 2014, retrieved from http://www.who.int/gender/whatisgender/en/